AI Chat Paper
Note: Please note that the following content is generated by AMiner AI. SciOpen does not take any responsibility related to this content.
{{lang === 'zh_CN' ? '文章概述' : 'Summary'}}
{{lang === 'en_US' ? '中' : 'Eng'}}
Chat more with AI
PDF (3.1 MB)
Collect
Submit Manuscript AI Chat Paper
Show Outline
Outline
Show full outline
Hide outline
Outline
Show full outline
Hide outline
Research Article | Open Access

Overactive alerting attention function in immigrants to high-altitude Tibet

 Plateau Brain Science Research Center, South China Normal University/Tibet University, Guangzhou 510631/Lhasa 850012, China
 Center for the Study of Applied Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science of Guangdong Province, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
 Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
 College of Management, Tianjin University, Tianjin 300072, China
 Department of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo 315211, China

§ Hailin Ma and Xinjuan Zhang contributed equally to this work.

Show Author Information

Graphical Abstract

Abstract

The present study combined the attention network test and event-related potential approaches to investigate the neurocognitive expression of resource reduction on attention function as a result of long-term high-altitude exposure in immigrants of Tibet. When compared with low-altitude residents, the study found that high-altitude exposure decreased executive-control behavioral performance but enhanced the alerting response. Correspondingly, changes in the target N2 and P3 amplitudes indicated a decrease in conflict inhibition underlying the executive-control network. Instead, the study noted that high-altitude exposure induced additional attentional resources to the alerting stage from the aspect of a change in the cue/target N1 and P1 amplitudes, which may be derived from a reduced self-referencing function. Taken together, the current findings provided experimental evidence for the tight relationship between reduced general cognitive inhibition to the hypersensitivity of the altering attention network to external stimuli mainly observed in immigrants to Tibet.

References

[1]
Singh, S., Thakur, L., Anand, J., Yadav, D. Effect of chronic hypobaric hypoxia on components of the human event related potential. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 2004, 120(2): 9499.
[2]
Thakur, L., Ray, K., Anand, J., Panjwani, U. Event related potential (ERP) P300 after 6 months residence at 4115 meter. The Indian Journal of Medical Research, 2011, 134(1): 113117.
[3]
Ma, H. L., Wang, Y., Wu, J. H., Wang, B. X., Guo, S. C., Luo, P., Han, B. X. Long-term exposure to high altitude affects conflict control in the conflict-resolving stage. PLoS One, 2015, 10(12): e0145246.
[4]
Posner, M. I. Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1980, 32(1): 325.
[5]
Wang, Y., Ma, H. L., Fu, S. M., Guo, S. C., Yang, X. F., Luo, P., Han, B. X. Long-term exposure to high altitude affects voluntary spatial attention at early and late processing stages. Scientific Reports, 2015, 4(1): 4443.
[6]
Richardson, C., Hogan, A. M., Bucks, R. S., Baya, A., Virues-Ortega, J., Holloway, J. W., Rose-Zerilli, M., Palmer, L. J., Webster, R. J., Kirkham, F. J. et al. Neurophysiological evidence for cognitive and brain functional adaptation in adolescents living at high altitude. Clinical Neurophysiology, 2011, 122(9): 17261734.
[7]
Hayashi, R., Matsuzawa, Y., Kubo, K., Kobayashi, T. Effects of simulated high altitude on event-related potential (P300) and auditory brain-stem responses. Clinical Neurophysiology, 2005, 116(6): 14711476.
[8]
Hackett, P. H., Roach, R. C. High-altitude illness. New England Journal of Medicine, 2001, 345(2): 107114.
[9]
Stivalet, P., Leifflen, D., Poquin, D., Savourey, G. Positive expiratory pressure as a method for preventing the impairment of attentional processes by hypoxia. Ergonomics, 2000, 43(4): 474485.
[10]
Berry, D. T. R., McConnell, J. W., Phillips, B. A., Carswell, C. M., Lamb, D. G., Prine, B. C. Isocapnic hypoxemia and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 1989, 11(2): 241251.
[11]
Evans, W. O., Witt, N. F. The interaction of high altitude and psychotropic drug action. Psychopharmacologia, 1966, 10(2): 184188.
[12]
Pun, M., Guadagni, V., Bettauer, K. M., Drogos, L. L., Aitken, J. A., Hartmann, S. E., Furian, M., Muralt, L., Lichtblau, M., Bader, P. R. et al. Effects on cognitive functioning of acute, subacute and repeated exposures to high altitude. Frontiers in Physiology, 2018, 9: 1131.
[13]
Chiu, G., Chatterjee, D., Johnson, R. W., Freund, G. G. The impact of acute hypoxia on learning and memory. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 2010, 24: S40.
[14]
Bonnon, M., No?l-Jorand, M. C., Therme, P. Effects of different stay durations on attentional performance during two mountain expeditions. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 2000, 71(7): 678684.
[15]
Wesensten, N. J., Crowley, J., Balkin, T., Kamimori, G., Iwanyk, E., Pearson, N., Devine, J., Belenky, G., Cymerman, A. Effects of simulated high altitude exposure on long-latency event-related brain potentials and performance. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1993, 64(1): 3036.
[16]
Swerdlow, N., Geyer, M., Braff, D. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: Current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology, 2001, 156(2–3): 194215.
[17]
Yan, X. D., Zhang, J. X., Gong, Q. Y., Weng, X. C. Prolonged high-altitude residence impacts verbal working memory: An fMRI study. Experimental Brain Research, 2011, 208(3): 437445.
[18]
Ma, H. L., Huang, X. Y., Liu, M., Ma, H. F., Zhang, D. L. Aging of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attentional processes in young immigrants with long-term high altitude exposure in Tibet: An ERP study. Scientific Reports, 2018, 8(1): 17417.
[19]
Zhang, D. L., Ma, H. L., Huang, J. Q., Zhang, X. J., Ma, H. F., Liu, M. Exploring the impact of chronic high-altitude exposure on visual spatial attention using the ERP approach. Brain and Behavior, 2018, 8(5): e00944.
[20]
Zhang, D. L., Zhang, X. J., Ma, H. L., Wang, Y., Ma, H. F., Liu, M. Competition among the attentional networks due to resource reduction in Tibetan indigenous residents: Evidence from event-related potentials. Scientific Reports, 2018, 8(1): 110.
[21]
Hochachka, P. W., Clark, C. M., Brown, W. D., Stanley, C., Stone, C. K., Nickles, R. J., Zhu, G. G., Allen, P. S., Holden, J. E. The brain at high altitude: Hypometabolism as a defense against chronic hypoxia? Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 1994, 14(4): 671679.
[22]
Yan, X. D., Zhang, J. X., Gong, Q. Y., Weng, X. C. Adaptive influence of long term high altitude residence on spatial working memory: An fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 2011, 77(1): 5359.
[23]
Petersen, S. E., Posner, M. I. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2012, 35(1): 7389.
[24]
Eimer, M. Mechanisms of visuospatial attention: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Visual Cognition, 1998, 5(1–2): 257286.
[25]
Brown, J.W. Beyond conflict monitoring: Cognitive control and the neural basis of thinking before you act. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2013, 22(3): 179185.
[26]
Groom, M. J., Cragg, L. Differential modulation of the N2 and P3 event-related potentials by response conflict and inhibition. Brain and Cognition, 2015, 97: 19.
[27]
Neuhaus, A. H., Urbanek, C., Opgen-Rhein, C., Hahn, E., Ta, T. M. T., Koehler, S., Gross, M., Dettling, M. Event-related potentials associated with Attention Network Test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 2010, 76(2): 7279.
[28]
Yang, T. T., Xiang, L. Executive control dysfunction in subclinical depressive undergraduates: Evidence from the Attention Network Test. Journal of Affective Disorders, 2019, 245: 130139.
[29]
Williams, R. S., Biel, A. L., Wegier, P., Lapp, L. K., Dyson, B. J., Spaniol, J. Age differences in the Attention Network Test: Evidence from behavior and event-related potentials. Brain and Cognition, 2016, 102: 6579.
[30]
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., Posner, M. I. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2002, 14(3): 340347.
[31]
Fan, J., Byrne, J., Worden, M. S., Guise, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Posner, M. I. The relation of brain oscillations to attentional networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 2007, 27(23): 61976206.
[32]
Fleck, J. I., Payne, L., Halko, C., Purcell, M. Should we pay attention to eye movements? The impact of bilateral eye movements on behavioral and neural responses during the Attention Network Test. Brain and Cognition, 2019, 132: 5671.
[33]
Neuhaus, A. H., Urbanek, C., Opgen-Rhein, C., Hahn, E., Ta, T. M. T., Koehler, S., Gross, M., Dettling, M. Event-related potentials associated with Attention Network Test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 2010, 76(2): 7279.
[34]
Wagner, M., Fuchs, M., Kastner, J. Evaluation of sLORETA in the presence of noise and multiple sources. Brain Topography, 2004, 16(4): 277280.
[35]
Zhang, D. L., Liu, X., Chen, J., Liu, B., Wang, J. H. Widespread increase of functional connectivity in Parkinson's disease with tremor: A resting-state FMRI study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2015, 7: 6.
[36]
Ma, H. L., Wang, Y., Wu, J. H., Luo, P., Han, B. X. Long-term exposure to high altitude affects response inhibition in the conflict-monitoring stage. Scientific Reports, 2015, 5: 13701.
[37]
Ma, H. L., Wang, Y., Wu, J. H., Liu, H. L., Luo, P., Han, B. X. Overactive performance monitoring resulting from chronic exposure to high altitude. Aerospace Medicine and Human Performance, 2015, 86(10): 860864.
[38]
Nakata, H., Sakamoto, K., Kakigi, R. Characteristics of No-go-P300 component during somatosensory Go/No-go paradigms. Neuroscience Letters, 2010, 478(3): 124127.
[39]
Doallo, S., Lorenzo-López, L., Vizoso, C., Rodríguez Holguín, S., Amenedo, E., Bará, S., Cadaveira, F. The time course of the effects of central and peripheral cues on visual processing: An event-related potentials study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 2004, 115(1): 199210.
[40]
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., Shulman, G. L. A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 2001, 98(2): 676682.
[41]
de Pisapia, N., Barchiesi, G., Jovicich, J., Cattaneo, L. The role of medial prefrontal cortex in processing emotional self-referential information: A combined TMS/fMRI study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 2019, 13(3): 603614.
[42]
Blakemore, S. J. The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2008, 9(4): 267277.
[43]
Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Fink, G. R., Vogeley, K. Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Consciousness and Cognition, 2008, 17(2): 457467.
[44]
Simpson, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Gusnard, D. A., Raichle, M. E. Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex: I. During cognitive task performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 2001, 98(2): 683687.
[45]
Xu, R., Yang, J., Feng, C. L., Wu, H. Y., Huang, R. W., Yang, Q. L., Li, Z. H., Xu, P. F., Gu, R. L., Luo, Y. J. Time is nothing: Emotional consistency of autobiographical memory and its neural basis. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 2018, 12(4): 10531066.
[46]
Korte, S. M., Koolhaas, J. M., Wingfield, J. C., McEwen, B. S. The Darwinian concept of stress: Benefits of allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2005, 29(1): 338.
[47]
Swerdlow, N. R., Caine, S. B., Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A. The neural substrates of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: A review of recent findings and their implications. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England), 1992, 6(2): 176190.
[48]
Frost, W. N., Tian, L. M., Hoppe, T. A., Mongeluzi, D. L., Wang, J. A cellular mechanism for prepulse inhibition. Neuron, 2003, 40(5): 9911001.
[49]
Nusbaum, M. P., Contreras, D. Sensorimotor gating: Startle submits to presynaptic inhibition. Current Biology, 2004, 14(6): R247R249.
[50]
Rose, P. K., Scott, S. H. Sensory-motor control: A long-awaited behavioral correlate of presynaptic inhibition. Nature Neuroscience, 2003, 6(12): 12431245.
[51]
Geyer, M. A., Krebs-Thomson, K., Braff, D. L., Swerdlow, N. R. Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: A decade in review. Psychopharmacology, 2001, 156(2–3): 117154.
[52]
Minassian, A., Feifel, D., Perry, W. The relationship between sensorimotor gating and clinical improvement in acutely ill schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research, 2007, 89(1–3): 225231.
Stress and Brain
Pages 76-95
Cite this article:
Ma H, Zhang X, Wang Y, et al. Overactive alerting attention function in immigrants to high-altitude Tibet. Stress and Brain, 2021, 1(1): 76-95. https://doi.org/10.26599/SAB.2020.9060008

2301

Views

233

Downloads

8

Crossref

Altmetrics

Received: 22 April 2020
Revised: 05 June 2020
Accepted: 22 July 2020
Published: 22 March 2021
© The Author(s) 2020

Creative Commons Non Commercial CC BY-NC: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission.

Return